Selasa, 24 Mei 2011

[D340.Ebook] Free PDF Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin

Free PDF Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin

Reviewing book Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin, nowadays, will not require you to always purchase in the store off-line. There is a fantastic area to buy the book Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin by on-line. This website is the most effective site with great deals varieties of book collections. As this Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin will be in this publication, all publications that you need will correct here, too. Merely look for the name or title of the book Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin You can find exactly what you are hunting for.

Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin

Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin



Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin

Free PDF Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin

Simply for you today! Discover your preferred publication here by downloading and also getting the soft file of guide Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin This is not your time to typically likely to guide stores to buy an e-book. Below, selections of book Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin and also collections are offered to download. Among them is this Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin as your favored e-book. Obtaining this publication Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin by online in this site can be realized now by checking out the web link web page to download. It will be simple. Why should be right here?

Reviewing routine will certainly constantly lead individuals not to completely satisfied reading Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin, a book, ten e-book, hundreds e-books, and more. One that will certainly make them feel satisfied is completing reviewing this e-book Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin as well as obtaining the message of the e-books, then discovering the various other following e-book to review. It proceeds a growing number of. The moment to finish reviewing a book Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin will certainly be consistently various depending on spar time to spend; one instance is this Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin

Now, how do you recognize where to get this book Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin Don't bother, now you could not visit the publication shop under the intense sunlight or night to look guide Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin We right here consistently aid you to discover hundreds sort of e-book. Among them is this publication qualified Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin You could go to the link web page provided in this collection and after that choose downloading. It will not take even more times. Just link to your internet accessibility and also you could access the e-book Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin on the internet. Obviously, after downloading and install Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin, you might not print it.

You can save the soft file of this publication Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin It will certainly rely on your downtime and also tasks to open up and also read this publication Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin soft data. So, you may not hesitate to bring this book Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved And Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, By Keith Devlin almost everywhere you go. Merely add this sot data to your gizmo or computer system disk to let you read every single time and also almost everywhere you have time.

Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin

If people are endowed with a "number instinct" similar to the "language instinct"—as recent research suggests—then why can't everyone do math? In The Math Gene, mathematician and popular writer Keith Devlin attacks both sides of this question.Devlin offers a breathtakingly new theory of language development that describes how language evolved in two stages and how its main purpose was not communication. Devlin goes on to show that the ability to think mathematically arose out of the same symbol-manipulating ability that was so crucial to the very first emergence of true language.Why, then, can't we do math as well as we speak? The answer, says Devlin, is that we can and do—we just don't recognize when we're using mathematical reasoning.

  • Sales Rank: #14039578 in Books
  • Published on: 2000-03
  • Original language: English
  • Dimensions: 9.25" h x 6.25" w x 1.25" l,
  • Binding: Hardcover
  • 328 pages

Amazon.com Review
For many, the mere word "mathematics" is enough to conjure memories of incomprehension at school, and fear and loathing ever afterward. Countless otherwise well-educated people see mathematics as the skeleton in their intellectual closet--the one key subject demanding a talent that they so obviously did not possess.

Or so it seems to anyone who has felt very much on the outside of the subject. British mathematician Keith Devlin is certainly on the inside, and in The Math Gene, he has wonderful news for everyone: we can all join him there. For Devlin argues that we all possess the ability to cope with mathematics--if only we recognize what's required. While a number of recent books, notably Stanislas Dehaene's The Number Sense, have focused on numerical ability, the scope of Devlin's book is much larger. He examines the evidence that we all possess, if not literally a gene, then at least an inherent ability not just for arithmetic but for real mathematics: algebra, calculus, and the rest. Devlin even puts forward a Darwinian explanation for the origin of this ability, based on the idea that being able to handle abstract ideas and relationships confers key evolutionary advantages.

Mathematics merely involves a relatively high level of abstraction--but one we can all cope with, if we work at it. "Doing mathematics is very much like running a marathon," writes Devlin. "It does not require any special talent, and 'finishing' is largely a matter of wanting to succeed."

In its wealth of wonderful examples supporting the central argument, The Math Gene bears comparison with Steven Pinker's The Language Instinct, and its plain common sense about this most misunderstood of subjects is inspirational. Thoroughly recommended for anyone seeking to rid their intellectual closet of the skeleton of mathematical "incompetence." --Robert Matthews, Amazon.co.uk

From Publishers Weekly
Recently, luminaries like Steven Pinker have shown lay audiences neat theories about how language works and how our "language instinct" evolved. In the same years, writers like David Berlinski have made higher math entertaining and accessible. Here, prolific math writer and NPR commentator Devlin (The Language of Mathematics) has joined these two strands of popular science writing. Using up-to-date cognitive psychology, along with the history of math, Devlin aims to unfold our "innate sense of number" and to show what it has to do with language. He also hopes, more ambitiously, to win readers over to his own hypothesis about how our language and math "instincts" arose. Experiments show that chimps, like us, "use symbols to denote numbers," though human toddlers are far better at it. Combining a number sense with symbolic abilities, we use abstractions to manipulate quantities, leading to arithmetic and potentially to calculus and number theory. After several stellar chapters devoted largely to psychology experiments, Devlin switches gears to higher math, giving examples of how abstract models describe concrete thingsAfrom rotating clock faces to rattlesnake skins. The book takes another sharp turn, into the stimulating but quite crowded field of hypotheses about how our brains came to be. While responsibly laying out several hypotheses, Devlin favors the idea that enhanced symbolic abilities let early hominids think "off-line," asking and answering "what if" questions about tools, predators, habitats or prey. Some may wish Devlin had written two booksAone about math and language, the other about language and evolution; the former would likely ace the latter. Most readers, though, will appreciate the broad, accessible syntheses he does provide. 35 illus. (Sept.)
Copyright 2000 Reed Business Information, Inc.

From Library Journal
This book is not about mathematics or genetics or why some people are good at math and others are not. Rather, Devlin (Goodbye, Descartes) asks and attempts to answer the question, "How and why did human beings evolve the ability to do mathematics?" His point is that mathematics is more than arithmetic. Real mathematics involves making logical arguments about abstract objects. Devlin briefly outlines Chomsky's theory that we are all born with "hard-wired" linguistic ability. He then explains that the mental process of making logical connections between abstract objects and the mental process needed to construct sentences have the identical structure. Thus, we can see that the genetic heritage that gives us all the ability to communicate by language also gives us the ability to do mathematics. I am convinced that Devlin is correct, and, if you read this book, you will be, too. For all math and science collections.DHarold D. Shane, Baruch Coll. of CUNY
Copyright 2000 Reed Business Information, Inc.

Most helpful customer reviews

8 of 8 people found the following review helpful.
Does not provide support for its main theses.
By M. Le Corre
I am a cognitive scientist who studies how children acquire number concepts and numerical language. Based on the last 30 years of research on the acquisition of numerical thought, I think there are good reasons to believe that math and language are intimately related. Having enjoyed parts of Devlin's "The Language of Mathematics", I had high hopes when I picked up this book. The first few chapters were promising. For example, I completely agree with Devlin that, contrary to what is implicitly assumed in the field, the Number Sense has little to do with mathematical thought. Being a complete math neophyte, I also enjoyed his chapter on the nature of mathematical thinking. Unfortunately, for the rest, I was quite disappointed to find many interesting theses, but little in the way of articulate reasons for believing that they may be correct. Rather, I found much to disagree with.

The main theses of this book are:

1) Mathematical thought is a type of linguistic thought.
2) The math gene is the genetic endowment for Universal Grammar.
3) Universal Grammar specifies the "fundamental language tree" - i.e., the basic X-bar tree consisting of an XP, which consists of a SPEC and an X-bar, which consists of an X which optionally combines with another XP.
4) During the first hundred of thousands, if not millions of years of evolution of the genus "homo", brain size grew rapidly. Given the cost of growing a bigger brain, it had to be very useful. Growing the brain was useful because it allowed homo to recognize more types (bananas, monkeys, palm trees, etc...)
5) Then, sometime around 200,000 thousand years ago full-blown language appears. How? With the emergence of "off-line thinking": the brain can self-trigger patterns of activity that used to have to be caused by external stimuli.
6) off-line thinking requires more than types; it requires representations of structural relations between them (e.g., cause-effect).
7) types + structural relations = syntax
8) being able to talk about relations between people is the same as being able to talk about mathematical relations

There are several problems with these theses. First, the author rarely considers alternatives. Take for example the argument that goes: a) the increase in brain size had to be useful to make up for its cost; b) being able to recognize types is useful; c) therefore, the brain grew because brain growth allowed homo to recognize more types.

This could be. The problem is that there are many other alternatives. It could be that brain growth allowed homo to have a larger working memory, or to store more mental images of individual objects and events, or to inhibit immediate desires in the interest of delayed gratification, etc...To show that the brain grew to represent more types, Devlin would have to consider some alternatives and show that the alternatives are wrong. However, far too little of that happens. Moreover, there are other sources of evidence that could support Devlin's thesis. For example, if evolution endowed us with the capacity to recognize multiple types, then type recognition should be innate - it should be available to young human babies. Much infant research suggests that indeed human infants do distinguish basic types - e.g., animate-inanimate. However, this is a far cry from the whole inventory of types Devlin seems to have in mind. More importantly, Devlin doesn't present other types of evidence that could support or disconfirm his hypothesis.

Unfortunately, this is typical of most other arguments in the book. Devlin presents arguments that are consistent with his theses, but rarely considers alternatives. Moreover, the evidence he considers is usually deplorably thin.

The other strain of problems comes from loose and usually erroneous analyses of mental representations. For example, Devlin characterizes syntax as the fundamental language tree, a characterization that finds much support in linguistic theory. But then later on syntax is said to be the same as representations of relations between types (or of the structure of the world). The fundamental language tree does not represent any contentful relations between objects or types. Syntax is pure form, not meaning. So the relation between the fundamental language tree and representations of the structure of the world escapes me. More generally, I don't see why Devlin dedicated a whole chapter to the fundamental language tree (which by itself is quite good) because he never goes back to that idea. Be that as it may, there is another problem with the thesis. Syntax is not a representation of structural relations of the physical or of the social kind. For example, consider these sentences: "Joe convinced Bob," "Joe kissed Bob," "Joe purchased a log" and "Joe burned a log". These sentences have the same syntactic structure: NP VP NP. However, they are about completely different types of relations. The first is about mental causation, the second is about contact, the third is about transfer, and the fourth is about physical causation. Clearly then, syntactic structure is not the same as representation of relations between objects and people.

This problem recurs in many the arguments of the second half of the book. For example, if off-line thinking evolved the way Devlin says it evolved (i.e., it is an internally generated simulation of on-line thinking), then the capacity to think about the past or the future does not necessarily follow from off-line thinking. On its own, a simulation of on-line thinking (thinking about what is here now), is no more a representation of the past or future than on-line thinking itself. To think about the past or the future, one needs to represent the structure of time. If no such structure is available in on-line thinking, then it cannot appear in a simulation of on-line thinking. The general problem is that, other than being able to run without an external cause, the simulation cannot have properties that are not in what is being simulated.

The latter point also holds for the book's final thesis - i.e., that our capacity for math grew out of our capacity to gossip - to talk about relations between people. Devlin's argument for this seems to be that all relations are equal. So if i can talk about relations between people then, necessarily, I can also talk about relations between geometric transformations or between numbers. But this is not so, at least, not patently. Love, argue with, hide from, embarrass, and flirt with are all social relations. Each of them has a particular content, as seen by the fact that each of them entails particular things (e.g., if A embarrassed B, then it is plausible that B's ego dropped temporarily but if A flirted with B, it is likely that A's ego enjoyed a temporary boost). Likewise, mathematical relations like SUCCESSOR, IDENTICAL, or SIMILAR have their particular content. How the content of any of these relations can be derived from social relations is quite unclear. To argue for his thesis that gossip is the origin of math, Devlin owes us an explanation of how one gets from the content of social relations such as embarass and flirt with, to relations like successor and identical. However, no such explanation is to be found. Therefore, as far as I can tell, there are no reasons to believe Devlin's final thesis. Rather, there are some pretty good ones to doubt it - i.e., that you cannot get representations of mathematical relations out of representations of social relations.

What Devlin really needs is evidence for abstract symbols that only capture the most basic logical properties of relations (say whether they are symmetrical or transitive). Here syntactic categories and the fundamental language tree could be part of the answer. But unfortunately Devlin does not make this connection.

7 of 8 people found the following review helpful.
A book about the origin of gossip
By Luke James
In the book The Math Gene, author Keith Devlin stated that math is just a special use of our language faculty and every one should be able to do math. The basic structure of the book is Prologue, what is math, what is language, and from language to math( the point). We can see this clearly from the last sentence of the prologue," Once you know what mathematics is really about, and once you see how our brains create language, you should find it far less surprising that thinking mathematically is just a specialized form of using our language facility."
He spends the first 4 chapters talking about what math is: the science of pattern.
Chapter 2 and 3 is mostly boring to me, maybe people with little math background will appreciate it. Chapter 4 is not bad, especially when he cited the research on animal coating, it is really interesting that the coating of a leopard could be generated by solving math equations. Other examples are also very interesting.

In chapter 5, the author talked about math thinking. It is abstract thinking. Then he used the house metaphor to compare it with daily life. Make it simple to understand for the readers. He also talked about the high concentration you need in doing math problems, this kind of concentration is very hard for ordinary people who are busy worrying about their daily life. And that��s one of major hurdle preventing most people from being good at math. This chapter is quite good.
Again chapter 6 and 7 is a bit uninteresting. It talked about what language is. Then in chapter 8 he talked about how math thinking evolved and the idea of offline thinking, how important it is to human, etc. It is an important chapter, though I don��t find it interesting.
The best part of the book for me, also the whole point of the book comes last in chapter 9. It tells "why numbers are like gossip". I am very interested to read about it.
It first cited a very common fact: that people like to talk about other people, also they are interested in other people��s lives, it��s like a need for them. Then he said the usefulness of this is that it will benefit the group, make its members more close to each other.
Then he said the same thinking involved could be used in math thinking. How relationship between people in real world could also be applied to the relationships between abstract objects in the imaginary world created by human brain. Then he said why most people can not do math even they have this faculty in them. It is because it takes training most people don��t have. Once those people get training, they will also be able to do math. However most people are not motivated.
And he said how mathematicians are able to do it and how wonderful the math world is. Only people that has reached the summit of one of the many math mountains can see the whole picture and the very picture motivate them to go further. While amateurs and outsiders only walk around the valley and never see the whole picture, and that is why they think math is difficult and uninteresting.
Chapter 10 is just citing about some other people��s opinion and his viewpoint on it.
The epilogue of the book " how to sell soap" is also quite funny.
Overall, it is a worth reading book. It is something new for me. Gives me more insight in language, math and gossip. The idea of the author is quite convincing. Some chapters of the book serve as background knowledge and could be skipped if you already have them. Mainly chapter 2, 3, 6, also chapter 10 could be skipped.

3 of 3 people found the following review helpful.
A Book about the Origin of Gossip
By Luhua Jiao
In the book The Math Gene, author Keith Devlin stated that math is just a special use of our language faculty and every one should be able to do math. The basic structure of the book is Prologue, what is math, what is language, and from language to math( the point). We can see this clearly from the last sentence of the prologue,�� Once you know what mathematics is really about, and once you see how our brains create language, you should find it far less surprising that thinking mathematically is just a specialized form of using our language facility.��
He spends the first 4 chapters talking about what math is: the science of pattern.
Chapter 2 and 3 is mostly boring to me, maybe people with little math background will appreciate it. Chapter 4 is not bad, especially when he cited the research on animal coating, it is really interesting that the coating of a leopard could be generated by solving math equations. Other examples are also very interesting.

In chapter 5, the author talked about math thinking. It is abstract thinking. Then he used the house metaphor to compare it with daily life. Make it simple to understand for the readers. He also talked about the high concentration you need in doing math problems, this kind of concentration is very hard for ordinary people who are busy worrying about their daily life. And that��s one of major hurdle preventing most people from being good at math. This chapter is quite good.
Again chapter 6 and 7 is a bit uninteresting. It talked about what language is. Then in chapter 8 he talked about how math thinking evolved and the idea of offline thinking, how important it is to human, etc. It is an important chapter, though I don��t find it interesting.
The best part of the book for me, also the whole point of the book comes last in chapter 9. It tells ��why numbers are like gossip��. I am very interested to read about it.
It first cited a very common fact: that people like to talk about other people, also they are interested in other people��s lives, it��s like a need for them. Then he said the usefulness of this is that it will benefit the group, make its members more close to each other.
Then he said the same thinking involved could be used in math thinking. How relationship between people in real world could also be applied to the relationships between abstract objects in the imaginary world created by human brain. Then he said why most people can not do math even they have this faculty in them. It is because it takes training most people don��t have. Once those people get training, they will also be able to do math. However most people are not motivated.
And he said how mathematicians are able to do it and how wonderful the math world is. Only people that has reached the summit of one of the many math mountains can see the whole picture and the very picture motivate them to go further. While amateurs and outsiders only walk around the valley and never see the whole picture, and that is why they think math is difficult and uninteresting.
Chapter 10 is just citing about some other people��s opinion and his viewpoint on it.
The epilogue of the book �� how to sell soap�� is also quite funny.
Overall, it is a worth reading book. It is something new for me. Gives me more insight in language, math and gossip. The idea of the author is quite convincing. Some chapters of the book serve as background knowledge and could be skipped if you already have them. Mainly chapter 2, 3, 6, also chapter 10 could be skipped.

See all 38 customer reviews...

Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin PDF
Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin EPub
Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin Doc
Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin iBooks
Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin rtf
Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin Mobipocket
Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin Kindle

Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin PDF

Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin PDF

Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin PDF
Math Gene: How Mathematical Thinking Evolved and Why Numbers Are Like Gossip, by Keith Devlin PDF

Tidak ada komentar:

Posting Komentar